Article/Report  |   April 2008
Lexical Representations in Children With SLI: Evidence From a Frequency-Manipulated Gating Task
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Elina Mainela-Arnold
    Pennsylvania State University
  • Julia L. Evans
    San Diego State University and University of California, San Diego
  • Jeffry A. Coady
    Boston University
  • Contact author: Elina Mainela-Arnold, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Pennsylvania State University, 401K Ford Building, University Park, PA 16802-3100. E-mail: ezm3@psu.edu.
Language Disorders / Specific Language Impairment / Language
Article/Report   |   April 2008
Lexical Representations in Children With SLI: Evidence From a Frequency-Manipulated Gating Task
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research April 2008, Vol.51, 381-393. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/028)
History: Accepted 03 Jul 2007 , Received 22 Jan 2007
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research April 2008, Vol.51, 381-393. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2008/028)
History: Accepted 03 Jul 2007 , Received 22 Jan 2007

Purpose: This study investigated lexical representations of children with specific language impairment (SLI) and typically developing, chronological age-matched (CA) peers on a frequency-manipulated gating task. The study tested the hypothesis that children with SLI have holistic phonological representations of words, that is, that children with SLI would exhibit smaller effects of neighborhood density on gating durations than CA peers and that children with SLI would be as efficient as CA peers in accessing high-frequency words but that they would differ from their age-matched peers in accessing low-frequency words.

Method: Thirty-two children (ages 8;5–12;3 [years;months]) participated: 16 children with SLI and 16 typically developing peers matched on age and nonverbal IQ. Children’s word guesses after different gating durations were investigated.

Results: Contrary to predictions, no group differences in effects of distributional regularity were found: Children in both groups required equally longer acoustic chunks to access words that were low in frequency and came from dense neighborhoods. However, children with SLI appeared to vacillate between multiple word candidates at significantly later gates when compared with children in the CA group.

Conclusions: Children with SLI did not exhibit evidence for phonologically holistic lexical representations. Instead, they appeared more vulnerable to competing words.

Order a Subscription
Pay Per View
Entire Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research content & archive
24-hour access
This Article
24-hour access

Related Articles

Children with a history of SLI show reduced sensitivity to audiovisual temporal asynchrony: An ERP Study
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research March 2014, Vol., No Pagination Specified. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0192
The relationship between mathematics and language: Academic implications for children with specific language impairment and English Language Learners
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools February 2014, Vol., No Pagination Specified. doi:10.1044/2014_LSHSS-13-0003
Differences in the performance of children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers on nonverbal cognitive tests: A meta-analysis
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research February 2014, Vol., No Pagination Specified. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-12-0363
Decreased Sensitivity to Long-Distance Dependencies in Children with a History of Specific Language Impairment: Electrophysiological Evidence
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research February 2014, Vol., No Pagination Specified. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0176
Production and processing of subject-verb agreement in monolingual Dutch children with Specific Language Impairment
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research February 2014, Vol., No Pagination Specified. doi:10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0104